Profile
International Journal of Physical Therapy & Rehabilitation Volume 3 (2017), Article ID 3:IJPTR-133, 8 pages
https://doi.org/10.15344/2455-7498/2017/133
Research Article
Attentional Demands of Locomotor and Balance Tasks in Individuals Aging with and without a Stroke

Prakruti J. Patel and Tanvi Bhatt*

Department of Physical Therapy, University of Illinois at Chicago, 1919 W. Taylor Street, Chicago, IL 60612, USA
Prof. Tanvi Bhatt, Department of Physical Therapy, Room729, University of Illinois at Chicago, 1919 W. Taylor Street, Chicago, IL 60612, Tel: 312 355 4443; E-mail: tbhatt6@uic.edu
14 March 2017; 14 June 2017; 16 June 2017
Bhatt T, Patel PJ (2017) Attentional Demands of Locomotor and Balance Tasks in Individuals Aging with and without a Stroke. Int J Phys Ther Rehab 3: 133. doi: https://doi.org/10.15344/2455-7498/2017/133
This study was supported by the American Heart Association, Scientific Development grant (12SDG12170022). The authors would like to thank Yashashree Apte, Savitha Subramanium for assistance with data collection and Harish Reddy and Juana Villagomez for assistance with data processing.

Abstract

Background: The purpose of this study was to examine the cognitive-motor interference pattern while concurrently performing a higher cognitive task with three dynamic postural tasks – limits of stability (intentional balance), compensatory stepping during (reactive balance -RB) and walking tasks across younger adults (YA), older adults (OA) and older chronic stroke survivors.
Methods: Thirty-six individuals in each group performed either of the three postural tasks – forward limits of stability (LOS, intentional balance), forward compensatory stepping (reactive balance - RB) and walking task (gait) with (dual-task) and without (single-task) performing a serial subtraction (SS) task. The SS tasks was also performed in isolation. Maximum center of pressure excursion (MXE) for LOS, center of mass position relative to base of support at step touchdown for RB, gait velocity and number of correct responses for SS task were measured. Motor and cognitive costs were calculated.
Results: The healthy adults showed higher motor cost for more dynamic gait and RB tasks compared with the LOS task however, motor costs were similar for all the tasks in the stroke group. Although the motor cost for OA and stroke groups were more than YAs for all postural tasks, motor cost was significantly greater in stroke group than OA group only for the LOS task.
Conclusion: The attentional demands are higher for postural task that are biomechanically unstable in healthy adults. Chronic stroke survivors show disproportionate ability to divide attention seen by higher motor cost during LOS task. Nevertheless, in chronic phase of recovery, stroke survivors may regain some dual-task function displaying similar strategies as healthy counterparts.


1. Introduction

A dual-task paradigm has been widely used to understand the attentional demands of balance and locomotor tasks. The view that maintaining stability during upright postural tasks may involve attentional resources stems from several empirical findings showing that the performance on the postural and/or cognitive task declines when the two are performed simultaneously [1-4]. This is referred to as cognitive-motor interference (CMI) [1]. These findings can be interpreted in the light of two main frame works - “capacity sharing” and “bottleneck” theories which assume that the central resources/ capacity is limited resulting in a processing delays when two tasks requiring similar neural pathways are performed concurrently.

While evidence shows that balance and locomotor tasks are attentionally demanding, the innate demands of the motor task itself could affect the performance in a dual-tasking situation. For example, attentional demands for reaching from a standing position involving voluntary action however, limited movement across different body segments on a stable base of support (BOS), could differ from the demands of walking involving movement across several body segments through a changing BOS. Further, attentional demands may vary depending on threat perceived and the level of experience/skill to perform a specific task. Thus, maintaining balance while walking on an even surface could incur lower attention demand than that while catching balancing during sudden disturbance causing slip or a trip. Limited studies have examined the differential attentional needs of balance activities. One study by Lajoie et al. [2] demonstrated longer reaction times to an auditory stimulus during static standing as compared with static sitting suggesting postural tasks with greater balance demands also needs greater attentional resources.

As such, cognitive load during balance tasks also affects younger and older adults differently. Sparrow et al. demonstrated that older adults have greater reaction times to a visual task while walking or greater decline in gait speed while performing a memory recall task as compared with younger adults [3,4]. A direct injury to the central nervous system such as a stroke, Parkinson’s disease or multiple sclerosis resulting in both motor and cognitive impairments also seems to affect dual-tasking ability as these individuals need to maintain balance in the face of reduced neural resources and sensorimotor impairments [5,6]. Cognitive and motor deficits occur with aging and neurological conditions. As presence of cognitive deficits affect motor abilities, it is essential to understand attentional demands of different balance tasks involving intentional and reactive balance in individuals aging with and without neurological conditions as well.

Therefore, the purpose of this study is to examine the influence of a single higher cognitive (working memory) task while concurrently performing three different dynamic postural tasks – limits of stability (intentional balance), compensatory stepping during large magnitude backward perturbations (reactive balance) and walking across healthy younger adults, older adults and older chronic stroke survivors. Motor and cognitive cost of performing the three postural tasks under dualtask conditions was measured. Based on the attentional demands incurred by the postural tasks, the motor costs, cognitive or both the costs will differ across the three balance tasks within all the groups. Further, motor and cognitive costs of performing the balance tasks would be greater among stroke survivors as compared with healthy adults.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1 Participants

Healthy young adults (18-30 years, N = 36), community dwelling chronic stroke survivors (50-70 years, N =36) and age-matched healthy adults (50-70 years, N = 36) participated in the study. The information about type of stroke was obtained from subjects’ physicians. Stroke survivors were included if they satisfied the following criteria 1) ability to walk 10m with a speed of ≥ 0.58 m/s without any assistive device i.e., least limited and unlimited community ambulators and 2) intact cognitive function determined by score of ≥ 26/30 on Montreal Cognitive Assessment scale. This test focuses on different aspects of cognitive functions such as orientation, attention, recall, working memory, and language and the stroke survivors were excluded in presence of any other acute or chronic medical conditions. Table 1 shows subject demographics for all the participants. Performance on measures of balance – Berg Balance Scale, motor impairments – Chedoke McMaster Stroke Assessment and physical activity levels using Rapid Assessment of Physical Activity was assessed for stroke survivors (Table 2). The healthy adults were excluded if they presented with any acute or chronic musculoskeletal, neurological and cardiopulmonary conditions. In addition to stroke survivors, younger and age-matched healthy controls were included to examine the effect of both aging and chronic stroke on CMI. The CMI pattern was assessed for all subjects performing one of the three different balance tasks.

table 1
Table 1: Mean (standard deviation) of subject demographics. There was no significant difference in age, height and weight between the young, age-match and stroke groups.
table 2
Table 2: Demographics and clinical outcome measures scores for chronic stroke survivors.

2.2 Intentional balance assessment

Intentional balance was assessed using the limits of stability test (LOS) by NeuroCom SMART Equitest for computed dynamic posturography. The subjects donneda harness and stood on the platform placing their feet on the force plates. The subjects’ center of pressure vector was projected on the screen in front of them in the form of a figure, known as “avatar”. The subjects attained the initial position to maintain the avatar in the center of the screen. Upon receiving an auditory cue, subjects moved towards the target in the forward direction as fast as possible. Subjects are asked to reach as close to the target as possible and hold the avatar in that position until the second auditory cue was heard while refraining from stepping or holding onto the surrounding box. After receiving a single familiarization trial, all the subjects performed this task in single-task and dual-task conditions.

Outcome: The maximum excursion of the center of pressure (MXE) in the forward direction was recorded [7]. This indicates the maximum distance up to which the individual can shift his/her center of mass outside the base of support without initiating a step, reflecting the individual’s limits of stability in the forward direction.

2.3 Reactive Balance Assessment

The subjects were exposed to trip-like perturbations from standing position on a motorized treadmill, ActiveStep (Simbex, Lebenon, NH). Initially subjects assumed a comfortable stance on the treadmill with their feet shoulder width apart. A harness donned prevented the participants’ knees from touching the treadmill in case of a fall. Prior to the testing session, the participants were presented with a familiarization trial wherein they were instructed to execute a natural response to recover their balance upon a sudden backward trip-like perturbation. This trial was presented to acquaint the participants with testing procedure. Following familiarization, perturbations were triggered at 16.75 m/s2 with a displacement of 20 cm.

2.4 Data collection and analysis

An eight camera motion capture system with a sampling rate of 120 Hz recorded full body kinematics (Motion Analysis Corporation, Santa Rosa, CA). The Helen Hayes marker set with 29 markers placed on bilateral bony landmarks, head and trunk was used to compute the joint centers and center of mass (COM) [8]. An additional marker was placed on the treadmill belt to identify the instant of perturbation onset. The raw marker data were low pass filtered using the fourth order Butterworth filter with a cut off frequency of 6Hz. The kinematic variables were computed using custom written algorithms in MATLAB version 2014b (The MathWorks Inc, Nactick, MA).

Outcome: The center of mass (COM) position was recorded relative to the anterior margin of the base of support (BOS) at touchdown of the stepping limb in the anteroposterior direction (XCOM/BOS) and normalized to the individual’s foot length. A more positive XCOM/ BOS would indicate greater instability in the forward direction. The XCOM/BOSwas recorded touchdown as it would represent the individual’s stability at the instance when they re-establish BOS after the initial balance loss at liftoff.

2.5 Gait assessment

Gait parameters were recorded using an electronic mat GaitRite (CIR Systems, Inc., Sparta, NJ). It consists of sensors embedded into 12 x 2 feet mat which measures spatial and temporal gait parameters via the accompanying GaitRite software (GaitRite Gold, Version 3.2). To record the steady state walking pattern, subjects began walking about 1 meter before stepping on the mat and continued walking about 2 meters beyond the mat (Figure 1).

figure 1
Figure 1: Schematic representation of the three motor tasks – a) reactive balance (RB), b) limits of stability in the forward direction, c) gait. For the RB, subjects experienced a trip-like perturbation that evoked a compensatory stepping response. The center of mass position relative to base of support (XCOM/BOS) was recoded at compensatory step touchdown. During the gait task subjects walked on the GaitRite mat at preferred walking speed and the velocity was measured. The limits of stability (intentional balance task) required subjects to lean forward upon hearing an auditory cue. The maximum center of pressure (COP) excursion from the resting position (MXE) was recorded.

Outcome: Gait velocity was measured while the subjects walked on the mat and was defined as the distance walked in the walking time for that specific trial [9]. Gait velocity was used as the outcome measure as the effect of dual-tasking is reflected consistently on gait speed across studies in healthy adults [9,10] and in stroke survivors [5].

2.6 Cognitive task

Subjects performed a mental arithmetic or serial subtraction task involving counting backwards from a specific two-digit number by a given single-digit number. The number of correct responses over a period of 30s were recorded while standing (single-task) and while performing the balance tasks (dual-task).

2.7 Experimental protocol

Subjects first received standardized instructions on how to perform the cognitive task followed by one familiarization trial. Subjects then performed a single trial of the cognitive task in standing position. This was by followed by random allocation of the subjects to evaluate dual tasking function on one of the three balance tasks i.e. either the LOS, gait or reactive balance tasks. Within each of the groups the balance task was performed in single-task i.e. performing the balance task without the cognitive task and dual-task conditions. The duration of each trial was 10-30s depending upon type of balance task.

  1. Limits of Stability (LOS): After a familiarization trial, the subjects performed the LOS task in isolation followed by performing the LOS tasks concurrently with the serial subtraction task. In the dual-task condition, subjects initiated the balance and serial subtraction task simultaneously upon hearing the auditory cue.
  2. Gait: Subjects initially walked for three trials at preferred walking speed followed by another block of three trials in the dual-task condition wherein the subjects began the serial subtraction walking tasks simultaneously.
  3. Reactive balance task (RB): Upon familiarization to this balance task, subjects were exposed to a single trip in absence of a cognitive task (single-task). Subjects then performed the balance task in dual-task condition. They were instructed to begin the cognitive task upon hearing a verbal prompt. The cognitive task and perturbation trial were initiated simultaneously. The a triplike perturbation was then triggered about 10-15s after initiating the trial.

For the LOS and reactive balance tasks, half of the subject performed balance tasks in the single-task condition prior to the dual-task and the other half of the subjects performed trials in the reverse order.

2.8 Motor cost

The effect of dual-tasking on both balance and cognitive parameters was assessed by comparing the absolute values for all balance, gait and cognitive variables between single- and dual-task conditions. To compare the effect of dual-tasking across the balance tasks between the three groups, the motor and cognitive dual-task cost was measured using following the formula [11].

[(Single-task — Dual-task)/Single-task]*100

In this study, the motor cost represented the performance on the balance measures in dual-task condition and the motor cost was calculated for each of the balance tasks. Similarly, cognitive cost represented the performance on the cognitive task in dual-task condition while performing each of the three balance tasks. Thus, positive value for motor and/or cognitive cost reduced performance on the individual balance task and/or cognitive task under dual-task condition where as a negative value for motor and/or cognitive cost indicated improved performance in the dual-task condition on the respective motor/cognitive task. The differential challenge of the balance tasks was determined based upon the motor cost under the respective dual-task conditions.

2.9 Statistical analysis

To analyze the effect of dual-tasking across the different balance tasks, between the groups, 3 x 3 two-way repeated measures ANOVA was performed for motor and cognitive costs with the balance tasks (reactive balance, gait and LOS) as within-groups factor and groups (young, age-match adults and stroke) as the between group factor. The significant interactions and main effects were resolved by independent t-tests. Further, independent t-tests were performed to examine the difference between motor and cognitive costs for each balance task within each group. The alpha level was set at p < 0.05. All the analyses were performed using SPSS 24.00 (IBM. Inc).

3. Results

3.1 Effect of dual-tasking on balance tasks

The effect of dual-tasking on balance measures was compared by calculating the motor costs. There was a significant interaction between the type of balance task and the groups for motor cost [F (2,62) = 2.66, p <0.05, Figure 2a). The effect of dual-tasking during different balance tasks was not the same for each of the groups.Within the groups the motor costs differed across the balance tasks [main effect of balance task, F (2,31) = 3.02, p = 0.05]. For the young adults, the motor cost for the LOS tasks was significantly lower as compared with the other two tasks (p < 0.05 for LOS vs. RB and for p < 0.01 LOS vs. gait). Similarly, among the age-matched adults, the motor cost tended to be lower for LOS tasks as compared with the other two tasks (p < 0.05 for LOS vs. RB and LOS vs. gait). Within the stroke group, although there was a trend towards higher motor cost during the LOS task as compared with the other two tasks, there was no significant difference in motor cost between the three tasks (p > 0.05 for all comparisons).

There was a significant main effect of group for the motor cost, [F (2,31) = 14.89 p = 0.00, Figure 2a]. For the RB task, the motor cost was significantly greater for the age-matched and stroke groups than that for young adults (p = 0.01 for age-match vs. young, p = 0.01 for stroke vs. young) however, there was no difference in motor cost between the age-matched adults and stroke survivors (p > 0.05). Similarly, for gait task the motor cost was significantly lower in younger adults than the other two groups (p = 0.02 for young vs. age-matched and p = 0.04 for young vs. stroke). There was no difference in motor cost for gait between stroke and age-matched adults (p > 0.05). For the LOS task, the motor cost was significantly different between all three groups – young adults< age-matched adults (p = 0.00), age-matched adults< stroke survivors (p = 0.00) and young adults< stroke survivors (p = 0.00).

With regards to the cognitive cost, which indicated the performance on the cognitive task in dual-task conditions, there was no significant interaction between the type of balance tasks and the groups [F (2, 31) = 1.56, p > 0.05]. Further, there was no significant main effect of balance tasks on the cognitive costs [F (2,62) = 1.90, p > 0.05]. However, a a significant main effect of group was observed on the cognitive cost of dual-tasking[F (2,31) = 3.58, p < 0.05, Figure 2b]. The cognitive cost for RB task did not differ between the groups (p > 0.05 for all comparisons). The cognitive cost for gait task was greater among age-matched adults and stroke survivors as compared with younger adults (age-matched and stroke > young adults, p < 0.05). For the LOS task as well, the cognitive cost was higher in stroke survivors and age-match adults than younger adults (age-matched and stroke > young adults, p < 0.05) with no significant difference in cognitive cost between age-matched and stroke survivors (p > 0.05).

figure 2
Figure 2: Differences in means (± SE) (a) motor cost and (b) cognitive cost across the balance tasks, RB = reactive balance, LOS = limits of stability between young adults, age-matched (AM) adults and stroke survivors. Significant differences between groups are indicated by the letters, same letters indicate no significant differences between those groups. The within group differences are indicated by *. Significance level was set at p< 0.05.

Further, a comparison of motor and cognitive costs for each of the tasks among all groups showed significantly greater cognitive cost for reactive balance task (p = 0.03) and LOS (p = 0.00) tasks among the younger adults with no significant difference between the costs for gait task (p > 0.05) (Figure 3a-c). The age-matched adults and stroke survivors showed a higher cognitive cost for LOS (p = 0.00 for agematch, p = 0.01 for stroke) however there was no difference between motor and cognitive costs for the RB and gait tasks (Figures 3d-i).

figure 3
Figure 3: Comparison of motor and cognitive costs for each of the balance tasks (RB = Reactive balance, LOS = Limits of stability) within young adults (a-c), age-matched adults (2-f) and stroke survivors (g-i). * indicates p < 0.05 and ** indicates p < 0.01.

4. Discussion

This study aimed to examine the whether the CMI pattern differed with regards to the type of balance task and effect of aging with and without a stroke on CMI pattern across the motor tasks. As hypothesized, the type of balance task influenced the CMI pattern.

Further, chronic stroke had some effect on the CMI pattern. Generally, the age-matched adults and chronic stroke survivors showed higher motor costs for all three tasks however, marked differences between all three groups were observed predominantly for the LOS task. A decline in cognitive performance was also observed among all the groups during dual-tasking.

4.1 CMI across balance tasks in healthy nervous system

The effect of dual-tasking differed between the motor tasks in both younger and older adults, such that the motor cost for RB and gait tasks were similar and greater than the LOS task. The differences in motor costs may arise from the neural processes involved in movement control and bio-mechanical demands of the tasks itself. The supplementary motor area, which is involved in movement planning and posture control is proposed to have a central role in intentional, self-initiated movements, as that observed during the LOS task [12]. Although gait and reactive balance tasks could be controlled by the central pattern generators in brainstem and spinal cord, a descending influence of cortical centers via cerebellum facilitates modulation of these postural tasks [13-15]. Further, walking and compensatory stepping tasks require coordinated movements between trunk and lower extremities to maintain balance over changing BOS. On the contrary, a voluntary reaching task such as LOS, involves controlling the upper body segment and ankle torque while maintaining balance on a constant BOS. Considering the involvement of cortical areas during a bio-mechanically less challenging voluntary task, it is possible that individuals could explicitly allocate attention to maintain stability on the LOS task. The neural processes modulating tasks that pose greater threat to stability are likely interfered with greater extent by concurrent working memory tasks resulting in higher motor costs. These findings are in line with a previous study that showed the attentional demands of postural tasks progressively increased from sitting, to standing, to walking observed by an increase in reaction time on an auditory task [2].

Dual-tasking often results in a trade-off of attentional resources between motor and cognitive tasks, and it is possible to minimize the cost of performing either of the tasks in the presence of explicit instruction [16,17]. We observed implicit prioritization of motor tasks during RB and LOS, as the cognitive cost was considerably higher than motor cost for these tasks (see figures 3a & 3c). It suggests that younger adults attempted to trade off the performance on the cognitive task to achieve stability during momentary balance loss whether voluntary (LOS) or involuntary (RB). During the gait task however, the two costs were similar indicating a mutual cognitive-motor interference (figure 3b). Considering that walking is a continuous task, individuals likely attempt to allocate equal attentional resources to both motor and working memory tasks affecting performance on both the tasks equally.

4.2 Effect of aging on CMI across balance tasks

Similar to young adults, age-matched older adults also showed modulation in attentional demands with higher motor cost during less stable RB and gait tasks than during the LOS task. However, the effect of aging was evident by higher motor costs for all balance tasks and higher cognitive cost for the gait and LOS tasks [18]. With the normal aging process, there is a decline in balance control due to reduced proprioceptive function, impaired synergistic contraction of muscles contributing to greater co-activation of muscles and delayed postural reflexes [19-21] as well as reduced cognitive functions like working memory and information processing speed [22-24], the cognitive functions deemed crucial for balance control [25]. Considering reduced central capacity with aging alongside declining neuromuscular function could possibly limit greater allocation of attentional resources to motor tasks, contributing to higher motor costs as seen in our study [26]. Unlike young adults, age-matched adults prioritized the motor task only during the LOS task (Figure 3f) as seen in previous studies [27]. They were unable to do so during the RB task, which in fact posed higher threat to balance than the LOS task. The decline in central capacity with aging also appears to interfere with the ability to prioritize the motor task when the relative demands of the balance task are greater.

4.3 Effect of stroke on CMI across balance tasks

Unlike healthy adults (younger and older), the stroke survivors failed to show a specific trend for the motor cost. The motor costs were similar for all three tasks within this group. Stroke survivors perhaps demonstrate a disproportionate ability to divide attention between motor and cognitive tasks compared with healthy age-matched controls. Although it is well-known that there is a decline in dualtask function post-stroke [28-33], most studies have reported CMI during a single motor task such as gait, quiet standing, or voluntary stepping. Furthermore, no study thus far has examined CMI during compensatory stepping from large perturbations which is impaired in this population [34,35]. Hyndman et al. (2006) compared the CMI during quiet standing and walking and observed that a concurrent cognitive task affected on walking speed but not anteroposterior way during standing, suggesting that a simple task like standing may not be affected by a cognitive task among chronic stroke survivors [36]. Similarly, we observed a decline in both motor and cognitive performance on all the motor tasks focused on dynamic balance. The fact that all dynamic balance tasks incurred equal attentional resources regardless of the differential postural challenge, it is possible that the ability to flexibly allocate attention to postural task remains affected in chronic phases of stroke.

In comparison with age-matched adults, the motor cost was greater for the LOS task whereas the costs were comparable between the groups for gait and RB task.As stroke affects higher cortical areas which predominantly influence movement control during voluntary balance tasks, a cortical injury during stroke could possibly affect the ability to allocate attention to carry out voluntary tasks under cognitive loads. This may explain a higher motor cost for the voluntary, LOS task in stroke survivors as compared with the age-matched controls despite implicit prioritization of the motor task (see figure 3i) [27]. Considering that the stroke survivors in this study were in chronic phase of recovery (at least 3 years post stroke), it may be difficult to conclude that the CMI observed in this population may be directly due the stroke-related pathology [5].

The similarities in motor and cognitive costs between age-match controls and chronic stroke survivors during RB and gait tasks could be potentially related to motor recovery and continued community ambulation in the chronic phase post-stroke. During community ambulation, the demand for dual-tasking ability is significant. Independent mobility in different community settings (e.g. grocery stores, hospitals, school, post office, gym etc.) may perhaps facilitate gains in balance or assist in developing compensatory strategies. The severity of motor impairment is negatively associated with community mobility and balance [37]. The stroke survivors in our study showed only mild to moderate levels of motor impairment (CMSA leg score 3-6/7), were community ambulators and were involved in light to moderate levels of physical activity on daily basis (RAPA1 score 4-6/7). This could possibly explain similar dual-task function in stroke survivors and age-matched healthy controls. Very few studies have investigated the CMI in chronic stroke survivors with their healthy counterparts. Two studies compared effect of dual-tasking in ambulatory older stroke survivors with > 3 years post stroke with healthy controls [5,38]. These studies also reported a similar decline in gait velocity and cadence in older chronic stroke survivors and healthy adults in dual-task condition suggesting that dual-task deficits in chronic stroke survivors may be, in part, due to aging.

Dual-tasking is increasingly incorporated in clinical settings for both assessment and training. At the rehabilitation level, our findings emphasize the need of testing cognitive-motor interference during varied postural tasks among people aging with and without a cerebral injury from stroke. Consequently, the dual-task training should incorporate specific balance tasks which individuals find more challenging for example stepping or walking. Given some degree of similarity in CMI between older healthy individuals and stroke survivors, it is possible that stroke survivors show similar gains in dual-tasking ability with training. This study extends the literature related to CMI by demonstrating the attentional demands of balance tasks that may pose differential threat to stability among healthy younger adult and adults aging with and without a stroke. Although in the chronic stage, stroke survivors may show similar CMI as their healthy counterparts for dynamic balance tasks such as compensatory stepping, quasi-static balance tasks requiring constant postural movement such as leaning forward may be affected differently by cognitive interference in this population as compared with age matched adults.

Competing Interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Abbreviations

ANOVA = Analysis of variance
BOS = Base of support
CMI = Cognitive-motor interference
COM = Center of mass
CoP = Center of pressure
LOS = Limits of stability
MXE = Maximum center of pressure excursion
RB = Reactive balance
XCOM/BOS = Center of mass position relative to base of support


References

  1. Plummer P, Eskes G, Wallace S, Giuffrida C, Fraas M, et al. (2013) Cognitive-motor interference during functional mobility after stroke: state of the science and implications for future research. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 94: 2565-2574 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  2. Lajoie Y, Teasdale N, Bard C, Fleury M (1993) Attentional demands for static and dynamic equilibrium. Exp Brain Res 97: 139-144 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  3. Sparrow WA, Bradshaw EJ, Lamoureux E, Tirosh O (2002) Ageing effects on the attention demands of walking. Human movement science 21: 961- 972 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  4. Li KZ, Lindenberger U, Freund AM, Baltes PB (2001) Walking while memorizing: age-related differences in compensatory behavior. Psychol Sci 12: 230-237 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  5. Canning CG, Ada L, Paul SS (2006) Is automaticity of walking regained after stroke? Disabil Rehabil 28: 97-102 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  6. Kizony R, Levin MF, Hughey L, Perez C, Fung J (2010) Cognitive load and dual-task performance during locomotion poststroke: a feasibility study using a functional virtual environment. Physical therapy 90: 252-260 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  7. Au-Yeung SS, Hui-Chan CW, Tang JC (2009) Short-form Tai Chi improves standing balance of people with chronic stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 23: 515-522 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  8. Davis BR ÕS, Tyburski D, Gage RJ (1991) A gait analysis data collection and reduction technique. Human Movement Science 10: 575-587 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  9. Al-Yahya E, Dawes H, Smith L, Dennis A, Howells K, et al. (2011) Cognitive motor interference while walking: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 35: 715-728 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  10. Smith E, Cusack T, Blake C (2016) The effect of a dual task on gait speed in community dwelling older adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Gait & posture 44: 250-258 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  11. Kelly VE, Janke AA, Shumway-Cook A (2016) Effects of instructed focus and task difficulty on concurrent walking and cognitive task performance in healthy young adults. Exp Brain Res 207: 65-73 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  12. Jahanshahi M, Jenkins IH, Brown RG, Marsden CD, Passingham RE, et al. (1995) Self-initiated versus externally triggered movements. I. Aninvestigation using measurement of regional cerebral blood flow with PET and movement-related potentials in normal and Parkinson's disease subjects. Brain 118: 913-933 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  13. Morton SM, Bastian AJ (2004) Cerebellar control of balance and locomotion. Neuroscientist 10: 247-259 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  14. Beloozerova IN, Sirota MG, Orlovsky GN, Deliagina TG (2005) Activity of pyramidal tract neurons in the cat during postural corrections. J Neurophysiol 93: 1831-1844 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  15. Yang JF, Gorassini M (2006) Spinal and brain control of human walking: implications for retraining of walking. Neuroscientist 12: 379-389 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  16. Yogev-Seligmann G, Rotem-Galili Y, Mirelman A, Dickstein R, Giladi N,et al. (2010) How does explicit prioritization alter walking during dual-task performance? Effects of age and sex on gait speed and variability. Phys Ther 90: 177-186 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  17. Remaud A, Boyas S, Lajoie Y, Bilodeau M (2013) Attentional focus influences postural control and reaction time performances only during challenging dual-task conditions in healthy young adults. Exp Brain Res 231: 219-229 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  18. Boisgontier MP, Beets IA, Duysens J, Nieuwboer A, Krampe RT, et al. (2013) Age-related differences in attentional cost associated with postural dual tasks: increased recruitment of generic cognitive resources in older adults. Neurosci Biobehavior Rev 37: 1824-1837 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  19. Allum JH, Carpenter MG, Honegger F, Adkin AL, Bloem BR (2002) Agedependent variations in the directional sensitivity of balance corrections and compensatory arm movements in man. J Physiol 542: 643-663 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  20. Maki BE, McIlroy WE (1996) Postural control in the older adult. Clin Geriatr Med 12: 635-658 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  21. Nardone A, Siliotto R, Grasso M, Schieppati M (1995) Influence of aging on leg muscle reflex responses to stance perturbation. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 76: 158-165 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  22. Cerella J (1985) Information processing rates in the elderly. Psychol Bull 98: 67-83. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  23. Morcom AM, Good CD, Frackowiak RS, Rugg MD (2003) Age effects on the neural correlates of successful memory encoding. Brain 126: 213-229 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  24. Park DC, Reuter-Lorenz P (2009) The adaptive brain: aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annu Review Psychol 60:173-196 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  25. Yogev-Seligmann G, Hausdorff JM, Giladi N (2008) The role of executive function and attention in gait. Mov Disord 23: 329-342 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  26. Pashler H (1994) Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. Psychol Bull 116: 220-244 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  27. Bhatt T, Subramaniam S, Varghese R (2016) Examining interference of different cognitive tasks on voluntary balance control in aging and stroke. Exp Brain Res 234: 2575-2584 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  28. Regnaux JP, David D, Daniel O, Smail DB, Combeaud M, et al. (2005) Evidence for cognitive processes involved in the control of steady state of walking in healthy subjects and after cerebral damage. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 19: 125-132 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  29. Bowen A, Wenman R, Mickelborough J, Foster J, Hill E, et al. (2001) Dualtask effects of talking while walking on velocity and balance following a stroke. Age Ageing 30: 319-323 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  30. Cockburn J, Haggard P, Cock J, Fordham C (2003) Changing patterns of cognitive-motor interference (CMI) over time during recovery from stroke. Clin Rehabil 17: 167-173 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  31. Plummer-D'Amato P, Altmann LJ, Saracino D, Fox E, Behrman AL, et al. (2008) Interactions between cognitive tasks and gait after stroke: a dual task study. Gait Posture 27: 683-688 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  32. Baetens T, De Kegel A, Palmans T, Oostra K, Vanderstraeten G, Cambier D (2013) Gait analysis with cognitive-motor dual tasks to distinguish fallers from nonfallers among rehabilitating stroke patients. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 94: 680-686 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  33. Melzer I, Goldring M, Melzer Y, Green E, Tzedek I (2010) Voluntary stepping behavior under single- and dual-task conditions in chronic stroke survivors: A comparison between the involved and uninvolved legs. J Electromyogr Kinesiol 20: 1082-1087 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  34. Mansfield A, Inness EL, Wong JS, Fraser JE, McIlroy WE (2013) Is impaired control of reactive stepping related to falls during inpatient stroke rehabilitation? Neurorehabil Neural Repair 27: 526-533 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  35. Salot P, Patel P, Bhatt T (2016) Reactive Balance in Individuals With Chronic Stroke: Biomechanical Factors Related to Perturbation-Induced Backward Falling. Phys Thera 96: 338-347 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  36. Hyndman D, Ashburn A, Yardley L, Stack E (2006) Interference between balance, gait and cognitive task performance among people with stroke living in the community. Disabil Rehabil 28: 849-856 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  37. Knorr S, Brouwer B, Garland SJ (2010) Validity of the Community Balance and Mobility Scale in community-dwelling persons after stroke. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 91: 890-896 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  38. Pohl PS, Kemper S, Siengsukon CF, Boyd L, Vidoni ED, et al. (2011) Dualtask demands of hand movements for adults with stroke: a pilot study. Top Stroke Rehabil 18: 238-247 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar] [PubMed]